Please wait a minute...
Acta Psychologica Sinica    2015, Vol. 47 Issue (9) : 1101-1110     DOI: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2015.01101
|
Neural Processing of Recollection, Familiarity and Priming at Encoding
YE Xiaohong1,2; CHEN Youzhen2; MENG Yingfang2
(1 School of Education and Music, Sanming University, Sanming 365004, China)  (2 Department of Psychology, Fujian Normal University, Fuzhou 350007, China)
Download: PDF(532 KB)   Review File (1 KB) 
Export: BibTeX | EndNote | Reference Manager | ProCite | RefWorks    
Abstract  

 

The distinction between neural mechanisms of explicit and implicit expressions of memory has been well studied at the retrieval stage, but less at encoding. Several studies employed a novel paradigm to measure explicit memory and priming-without-explicit memory in one test, and contrasted the neural signals of these two processes at the encoding stage via a Dm analysis. However, dissociations obtained in these studies are complicated because of the contamination from familiarity, a fast automatic process in which memory judgments can be driven by the increased fluency of reprocessing studied information. Familiarity is also a form of explicit memory but different from recollection. Growing evidence has indicated that familiarity-based recognition judgments might rely on the same process that supports implicit memory or priming. Therefore, it is necessary to concurrently acquire and compare the encoding processes yielding later recollection, familiarity and priming in a single test.
In this study, a two-stage forced-choice recognition test was adopted within the subsequent memory (Dm) paradigm, so as to simultaneously acquire neural correlates of recollection, familiarity and priming in a single test. During the study phase, participants were instructed to judge the color of each word. There were two stages during the forced-choice recognition test phase. In the first stage, two words (one from the study list, and one new) appeared concurrently, and participants were instructed to indicate the studied word. If they could not recognize the studied word, a guess was permitted to make a choice. In the second stage, a cue to make a confidence judgment appeared directly following the recognition response, and participants indicated whether the foregoing studied-selection was based on remembering, knowing or guessing. Here, “remembering” refers to the retrieval of specific details from the study phase supporting the recognition decision, “knowing” refers to the recognition supported by a weak feeling of familiarity with few details retrieved from the study phase, and “guessing” refers to “absolutely no feeling of memory” such that the stimulus in no way felt “old”. The study ERP data were then classified into four categories as “subsequent remembered” (later retrieved with detailed information), “subsequent known” (later retrieved with a feeling of familiarity)”, “subsequent primed” (later retrieved without conscious awareness) and “subsequent forgotten” (not retrieved). Differences in subsequent memory effects (Dm effects) were measured by comparing ERP waveform associated with later memory based on recollection, familiarity or priming with that associated with later forgotten items. In addition, interference during encoding was introduced in Experiment 2 to determine whether three Dm effects were different from Experiment 1. The interference task was to judge the orientation of arrow which appeared with word at the same time.
The results showed that, in Experiment 1, the recollection Dm effect involved a robustly sustained (onset at 400 ms) prefrontal positive-going Dm effect which was right-lateralized, and a later (onset at 800 ms) occipital negative-going Dm effect. Familiarity involved an earlier (300~400 ms) prefrontal positive-going Dm effect and a later (500~600 ms) parietal positive-going Dm effect. Priming involved a negative-going Dm effect which onset at 700 ms, mainly distributed over anterior brain sites. In Experiment 2 with an interference task during encoding, a similar priming Dm effect that was negative-going during 600~800 ms at central site, and a similar familiarity Dm effect that was positive-going during 600~800 ms at frontal site were still observed. However, there was no evidence of Dm effect associated with recollection.

Taken the two ERP results together, we inferred that there would be a sequence of components that represented cognitive processes underlying the encoding of verbal information into episodic memory, and separately supported later remembering, knowing and priming.

Keywords recollection      familiarity      implicit memory      explicit memory      Dm effect     
Corresponding Authors: MENG Yingfang, E-mail: 175695016@qq.com   
Issue Date: 25 September 2015
Service
E-mail this article
E-mail Alert
RSS
Articles by authors
Cite this article:   
YE Xiaohong; CHEN Youzhen; MENG Yingfang. Neural Processing of Recollection, Familiarity and Priming at Encoding[J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica,2015, 47(9): 1101-1110.
URL:  
http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/EN/10.3724/SP.J.1041.2015.01101     OR     http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/EN/Y2015/V47/I9/1101
[1] LEI Yi, XIA Qi, MO Zhifeng, LI Hong. The attention bias effect of infant face: The mechanism of cuteness and familiarity[J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2020, 52(7): 811-822.
[2] HUANG Fajie,MENG Yingfang,YAN Ying. The effects of retrieval interference on different types of implicit memory[J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2020, 52(5): 572-583.
[3] LIU Guixiong,JIA Yongping,WANG Yujuan,MAIHEFULAITI ·Kanji,GUO Chunyan. The bilingual L2 advantage in associative recognition[J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2019, 51(1): 14-23.
[4] Jingyuan LIN, Wuji LIN, Yingfang MENG. Effects of nicotine on implicit and explicit memory[J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2018, 50(9): 940-952.
[5] LI Tingyu, LIU Li, LI Yilin, ZHU Liqi. Preschoolers' selective trust and belief revision in conflicting situation[J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2018, 50(12): 1390-1399.
[6] LIN Wuji, MENG Yingfang, LIN Jingyuan.  Effects of interference on retrieval process in implicit memory[J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2017, 49(7): 897-908.
[7] JIA Yongping; ZHOU Chu; LI Lin; GUO Xiuyan. Recognition without cued recall (RWCR) phenomenon in Chinese characters: Effects of restudying and testing[J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2016, 48(2): 111-120.
[8] MAO Xinrui; XU Huifang; GUO Chunyan. Emotional Memory Enhancement Effect in Dual-processing Recognition Retrieval[J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2015, 47(9): 1111-1123.
[9] WANG Pei;CHEN Li; XIE Yiwen; ZHANG Qin. The Effect of Familiarity and Compatibility on Mental Representation of the Stereotype in Compound Social Categories[J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2015, 47(3): 375-388.
[10] WU Binxing; ZHANG Zhijun; SUN Yusheng. Facial Familiarity Modulates the Interaction between Facial Gender and Emotional Expression[J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2015, 47(10): 1201-1212.
[11] CHEN Yongxiang; ZHU Liqi. Predictors of Action Picture Naming in Mandarin Chinese[J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2015, 47(1): 11-18.
[12] LIU Yingjie;WEI Ping;DING Jinhong;GUO Chunyan. Age Differences in the Repetition Effect for Studied or New Objects[J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2014, 46(3): 321-330.
[13] MENG Yingfang. Neural Correlates at Encoding of Covert Face Recognition[J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2013, 45(9): 935-943.
[14] MENG Ying-Fang. ERP Dissociation and Connection between Implicit and Explicit Memory at Encoding[J]. , 2012, 44(1): 30-39.
[15] WANG Juan,ZHANG Ji-Jia,XIE Shu-Shu,YUAN Ai-Ling. Influence of Chinese Character Learning Combined with Dongba Pictograph on Children’s Chinese Orthographic Acquisition[J]. , 2011, 43(05): 519-533.
Viewed
Full text


Abstract

Cited

  Shared   
  Discussed   
Copyright © Acta Psychologica Sinica
Support by Beijing Magtech