Please wait a minute...
Acta Psychologica Sinica    2015, Vol. 47 Issue (1) : 11-18     DOI: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2015.00011
|
Predictors of Action Picture Naming in Mandarin Chinese
CHEN Yongxiang1,2; ZHU Liqi2
(1 School of Education Science, Shanxi University, Taiyuan 030006, China) (2 Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing 100101, China)
Download: PDF(348 KB)   Review File (1 KB) 
Export: BibTeX | EndNote | Reference Manager | ProCite | RefWorks    
Abstract  

Nouns and verbs are two main categories of content words, and they are learned early by children. It remains in hot debate regarding whether Mandarin Chinese is a verb-friendly language for children. However, few studies examined verb processing in Mandarin, and there were no standard experimental materials that are available for researchers. Therefore, it is unclear what factors may influence action picture naming in Mandarin and whether it is similar to the rules in object picture naming. Thirty-six adults participated in picture naming and 112 adults participated in rating tasks. All participants were native speakers of Mandarin Chinese. The materials were 275 pictures from the IPNP website (see http://crl.ucsd.edu/experiments/ipnp/) (Szekely et al., 2004), of which 10 pictures were deleted because Chinese adults could not name them correctly. Chinese verb names, naming latency, name agreement, H value, familiarity, visual complexity, image agreement, and oral age of acquisition (AoA) were obtained for the 265 action pictures. Results from stepwise regression analysis showed that H value familiarity and visual complexity explained 72.4% of the variances in picture naming latency, whereas other variables such as word frequency and AoA were excluded from the final model. Moreover, the pictures were categorized into five levels of difficulty based on the naming latency. A comparison of the present study to previous studies that examined object picture naming in Mandarin showed that action picture naming (with a mean latency of 1617 ms) was more difficult than object picture naming (with a mean latency of 1324 ms or 1044 ms, Liu, Hao, Li, & Shu, 2011; Zhang & Yang, 2003). In addition, name agreement of action pictures was lower than that of object pictures, and visual complexity of action pictures was higher than that of object pictures. These results indicated that action picture naming was difficult for adult participants. One possibility is that it might be particularly difficult for Mandarin speakers to extract meaning out of the static action pictures and verbalize them. More investigations are needed to explore the mechanisms of verb processing in Mandarin Chinese, and the measures obtained from the present study can provide valuable tools for future researchers to examine verb processing in Mandarin.

Keywords Chinese verbs      picture naming      familiarity      name agreement      H value     
Corresponding Authors: ZHU Liqi, E-mail: zhulq@psych.ac.cn    
Issue Date: 26 January 2015
Service
E-mail this article
E-mail Alert
RSS
Articles by authors
Cite this article:   
CHEN Yongxiang; ZHU Liqi. Predictors of Action Picture Naming in Mandarin Chinese[J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica,2015, 47(1): 11-18.
URL:  
http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/EN/10.3724/SP.J.1041.2015.00011     OR     http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/EN/Y2015/V47/I1/11
[1] LEI Yi, XIA Qi, MO Zhifeng, LI Hong. The attention bias effect of infant face: The mechanism of cuteness and familiarity[J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2020, 52(7): 811-822.
[2] LOU Hao,LI Cong,ZHANG Qingfang. Distinct effects of age of acquisition in Chinese object and action picture naming: An ERP study[J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2019, 51(2): 143-153.
[3] LIU Guixiong,JIA Yongping,WANG Yujuan,MAIHEFULAITI ·Kanji,GUO Chunyan. The bilingual L2 advantage in associative recognition[J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2019, 51(1): 14-23.
[4] LI Tingyu, LIU Li, LI Yilin, ZHU Liqi. Preschoolers' selective trust and belief revision in conflicting situation[J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2018, 50(12): 1390-1399.
[5] JIA Yongping; ZHOU Chu; LI Lin; GUO Xiuyan. Recognition without cued recall (RWCR) phenomenon in Chinese characters: Effects of restudying and testing[J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2016, 48(2): 111-120.
[6] YE Xiaohong; CHEN Youzhen; MENG Yingfang. Neural Processing of Recollection, Familiarity and Priming at Encoding[J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2015, 47(9): 1101-1110.
[7] MAO Xinrui; XU Huifang; GUO Chunyan. Emotional Memory Enhancement Effect in Dual-processing Recognition Retrieval[J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2015, 47(9): 1111-1123.
[8] WANG Pei;CHEN Li; XIE Yiwen; ZHANG Qin. The Effect of Familiarity and Compatibility on Mental Representation of the Stereotype in Compound Social Categories[J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2015, 47(3): 375-388.
[9] WU Binxing; ZHANG Zhijun; SUN Yusheng. Facial Familiarity Modulates the Interaction between Facial Gender and Emotional Expression[J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2015, 47(10): 1201-1212.
[10] CHEN Yongxiang;ZHU Liqi. Associations of Body Parts and Early-Learned Mandarin Verbs and Their Effect on AoA of These Verbs[J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2014, 46(7): 912-921.
[11] ZHANG Ji-Jia,ZHANG Feng-Ling.

The Asymmetric Effect of Bilingualism and Diglossia on Picture Naming and Picture Classification

[J]. , 2010, 42(04): 452-466.
[12] CHEN Xu–Hai,HUANG Xi-Ting. The Encoding of Constituent Morphemes in the Oral Production of Chinese Disyllable Compound Words[J]. , 2010, 42(03): 377-386.
[13] Ning Ning,Lu Chunming,Peng Danling,Ding Guosheng. The Speech Plan Deficit of People who stutter: Evidence from Word Length Effect[J]. , 2007, 39(02): 215-224.
[14] Zhang-Jijia,Kong Changfeng. Effects of Categorical Variables on Fale Recognition[J]. , 2006, 38(03): 324-332.
[15] Zhang Jijia, Chen Xinkui. THE ROLE OF CHINESE CHARACTERS’ YIFU IN COGNITION OF CHINESE ACTION VERBS’ MEANING[J]. , 2005, 37(04): 434-441.
Viewed
Full text


Abstract

Cited

  Shared   
  Discussed   
Copyright © Acta Psychologica Sinica
Support by Beijing Magtech