Please wait a minute...
Acta Psychologica Sinica
|
The Development of Allocation of Study Time on Part-list Cuing Effect of Pictures
TANG Weihai;LIU Tuanli;SHI Ying;FENG Hong;LIU Xiping
(1 College of Education, Tianjin Normal University, Tianjin 300387, China) (2 Academy of Psychology and Behavior, Tianjin Normal University, Tianjin 300074, China)
Download: PDF(706 KB)   Review File (1 KB) 
Export: BibTeX | EndNote | Reference Manager | ProCite | RefWorks    
Abstract  

When people are asked to recall words they have studied earlier from a list, those given a subset of these words as cues recall fewer words than people who do not receive any cues. This phenomenon is the so called part-list cuing effect. This phenomenon seems to be alien from the general thought that cues can help to prompt memories. The current research examined the part-list cuing effect of participants of different ages in learning pictures, and also explored their memory monitoring and control. Adopting 3 experiments, we explored the development of part-list cuing effect of pictures, and the allocation of study time of different ages. Experiments 1 explored whether retrieval cues would have detrimental effects on participants’ recall of the remaining items after learning pictures, and Experiments 2 explored the performance of students in Grade 2 and Grade 5 of a primary school, Grade 2 of a junior high school, Grade 2 of a senior high school and Grade 2 of a university. Experiments 3 studied the development of memory control of different ages. The results showed that: (1) After participants learned a list of pictures, the presentation of a subset of learned pictures’ names as retrieval cues didn’t have detrimental effects on their recall of the remaining items significantly; (2) After participants learned a list of pictures, the presentation of a subset of learned pictures as retrieval cues had detrimental effects on their recall of the remaining items, that is to say, part-list cuing effect occurred; (3) The part-list cuing effect occurred in all age selected. And with the increase of age, its amount changed as an inverted U shape; (4) Participants’ allocation of study time was affected by the times they had practiced with part-list cueing, which was different among participants of different ages. Grade 2 of primary school students couldn’t predict the detrimental effect of part-list cues no matter how many times they had practiced. After practicing twice, Grade 5 of primary school students could realize part-list cues’ detrimental effect and effectively allocate their cognitive resources. And only after one practice, Grade 2 of junior high school and senior high school students could realize that the part-list cuing recall was more difficult compared to free recall and allocate their study time appropriately. Moreover, students in Grade 2 of senior high school did even better than those in junior high school. The present findings revealed that in the picture studying, cues presented by way of cross-domain is a boundary condition of part-list cueing effect, while age is not a boundary condition of part-list cueing effect. The development of children’s strategy on allocation of study time had a great change from Grade 2 of primary school to Grade 5 of primary school students, while the learning efficiency had a great change from Grade 2 of junior high school to Grade 2 of senior high school students.

Keywords part-list cuing effect of pictures      allocation of study time      experimenter-paced study      self-paced study      development     
Corresponding Authors: LIU Xiping   
Issue Date: 24 May 2014
Service
E-mail this article
E-mail Alert
RSS
Articles by authors
TANG Weihai
LIU Tuanli
SHI Ying
FENG Hong
LIU Xiping
Cite this article:   
TANG Weihai,LIU Tuanli,SHI Ying, et al. The Development of Allocation of Study Time on Part-list Cuing Effect of Pictures[J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2014.00621
URL:  
http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/EN/10.3724/SP.J.1041.2014.00621     OR     http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/EN/Y2014/V46/I5/621
[1] DONG Shuyang; LIANG Xi; ZHANG Ying; WANG Zhengyan. Maternal positive parenting behaviors and toddler’s compliance: Prediction from maternal sensitivity in infancy and bidirectional effects of autonomy encouragement in toddlerhood[J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2017, 49(4): 460-471.
[2] LIANG Feifei; WANG Yongsheng; YANG Wen; BAI Xuejun. The modulation of reading level on the development of children’s eye movement characteristics: Evidence from 9- to 11-year-old children[J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2017, 49(4): 450-459.
[3] XING Shufen; LIANG Xi; YUE Jianhong; WANG Zhengyan. Multiple attachment relationships and the impacts on children’s socio-emotional development under the background of grandmother co-parenting[J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2016, 48(5): 518-528.
[4] ZHU Xu; HU Yue; JIANG Guangrong. The Developmental Patterns of Working Alliance in Counseling: Relationships to Therapeutic Outcomes[J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2015, 47(10): 1279-1287.
[5] ZHANG Lijin;ZHANG Zhenfeng. The Contribution of Dynamic Assessment to Screening Children with Mathematics Learning Disabilities[J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2014, 46(8): 1112-1123.
[6] ZHANG Bo; LI Jian; XU Chu; LI Yiming. The Developmental Differences of Problem Solving Ability between Intellectually-gifted and Intellectually-average Children Aged from 11-14 Years Old[J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2014, 46(12): 1823-1834.
[7] LIN Ou;WANG Zhengke;MENG Xiangzhi. Visual Perceptual Learning in Chinese Developmental Dyslexia[J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2013, 45(7): 762-772.
[8] LIU Junsheng;ZHOU Ying;LI Dan. Developmental Trajectories of Loneliness During Middle and Late Childhood: A Latent Growth Curve Analysis[J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2013, 45(2): 179-192.
[9] LIU Xiao-Cen,SANG Biao,DOU Dong-Hui. Explicit and Implicit Self-Enhancement as Functions of Interpersonal Contexts[J]. , 2011, 43(11): 1293-1307.
[10] ZHANG Li-Jin,CHEN Liang,FANG Fu-Xi. The Adaptation of Dynamic Test Using the Inventory of Piaget’s Developmental Task (IPDT): An Initial Validation and Application[J]. , 2011, 43(09): 1075-1086.
[11] LIU Dian-Zhi,HUANG Hui-Xin,JIA Feng-Qin,GONG Qian,HUANG Qi1, LI Xia. A New Sex-Role Inventory (CSRI-50) Indicates Changes of Sex Role among Chinese College Students[J]. , 2011, 43(06): 639-649.
[12] CHEN Liang,ZHANG Wen-Xin,JI Lin-Qin,CHEN Guang-Hui,WEI Xing,CHANG Shu-Min. Developmental Trajectories and Gender Differences of Aggression during Middle and Late Childhood[J]. , 2011, 43(06): 629-638.
[13] Shu-Chen Li, Martin Lö,vdén, Sabine Schaefer, Florian Schmiedek, Yee Lee Shing, Markus Werkle-Bergner, and Ulman Lindenberger. The Center of Lifespan Psychology at the Max Planck Institute for Human Development: Conceptual Agenda and Illustration of Research Activities[J]. , 2009, 41(11): 1102-1122.
[14] JIN Sheng-Hua,ZHENG Jian-Jun,XIN Zhi-Yong. The Structure and Characteristics of Contemporary Chinese Values[J]. , 2009, 41(10): 1000-1014.
[15] KE Jiang-Lin,SUN Jian-Min,LI Yong-Rui. Psychological Capital: Chinese Indigenous Scale’s Development and Its Validity Comparison with the Western Scale[J]. , 2009, 41(09): 875-888.
Viewed
Full text


Abstract

Cited

  Shared   
  Discussed   
Copyright © Acta Psychologica Sinica
Support by Beijing Magtech