Please wait a minute...
心理学报  2018, Vol. 50 Issue (2): 206-215    DOI: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2018.00206
  本期目录 | 过刊浏览 | 高级检索 |
 小学低年级儿童口语词汇知识的发展轨迹及其影响因素
 程亚华1;  伍新春2;  刘红云2;  李 虹2
 (1宁波大学心理学系暨研究所, 宁波 315211) (2北京师范大学心理学部, 应用实验心理北京市重点实验室, 儿童阅读与学习研究中心, 北京 100875)
 The developmental trajectories of oral vocabulary knowledge and its influential factors in Chinese primary school students
 CHENG Yahua1; WU Xinchun2; LIU Hongyun2; LI Hong2
 (1 Department of Psychology, Ningbo University, Ningbo 315211, China) (2 Research Center of Children’s Reading and Learning, Beijing Key Laboratory of Applied Experimental Psychology, Faculty of Psychology, Beijing Normal University, Beijing 100875, China)
全文: PDF(438 KB)   评审附件 (1 KB) 
输出: BibTeX | EndNote (RIS)       背景资料
文章导读  
摘要  以149名小学生为研究对象, 对其口语词汇知识进行了历时3年的5次追踪测试, 采用潜变量增长模型探索了小学1~3年级学生汉语口语词汇知识的发展轨迹, 并考察了语音意识、同形语素意识、复合语素意识和家庭社会经济地位对口语词汇知识发展轨迹的影响。结果发现:(1) 1~3年级学生口语词汇知识发展轨迹呈曲线形式, 其中前两年呈线性发展, 三年级时呈加速发展, 发展速度是前期发展的两倍, 起始水平和发展速度均存在显著的个体差异; (2)语音意识、同形语素意识、复合语素意识和家庭社会经济地位均可以正向预测学生口语词汇知识的起始水平; (3)只有同形语素意识和家庭社会经济地位可以正向预测学生口语词汇知识的发展速度。
服务
把本文推荐给朋友
加入引用管理器
E-mail Alert
RSS
作者相关文章
程亚华
伍新春
刘红云
李虹
关键词 口语词汇知识 发展轨迹 潜变量增长模型 家庭社会经济地位 语素意识    
Abstract: The oral vocabulary knowledge is a crucial factor for language development and school success for children, and develops rapidly in childhood. The present study was aimed to extend our current understanding of oral vocabulary knowledge development, at first by examining its growth from grades 1 to 3. Based on such findings, further question was asked about how to explain the individual differences in the growth rate of oral vocabulary acquisition. Therefore, the current research also investigated the additive effects of family socioeconomic status (SES), children’s phonological awareness, homograph awareness and compounding awareness on initial levels and growth rate in oral vocabulary knowledge. Participants were 149 children in grade one, who were followed up for three years from grade one entering to grade three. They were assessed on phonological awareness, homograph awareness, and compounding awareness at Time 1, oral vocabulary knowledge from Time 1 to Time 5. Family socioeconomic status was obtained from parents of these children. Latent growth modeling was conducted to examine: (1) children’s initial levels and growth rate in oral vocabulary knowledge over time; and (2) the predictive effects of family SES, phonological awareness, homograph awareness, and compounding awareness on both initial levels and growth rate in oral vocabulary knowledge. Unconditional latent growth modeling analyses revealed that children’s oral vocabulary knowledge increased in a non-linear trajectory during the follow-up period. In specific, (1) children showed individual differences in the initial levels and growth rate in oral vocabulary knowledge. (2) the initial level of oral vocabulary knowledge was not related to later growth rate. Conditional latent growth modeling was examined with family SES, phonological awareness, homograph awareness, and compounding awareness as time-invariant predictors on both initial levels and growth rate in oral vocabulary knowledge. It was found that (1) children who have higher level of phonological awareness, homograph awareness, and compounding awareness had higher initial levels of oral vocabulary knowledge than others did (β = 0.15, p < 0.05; β = 0.28, p < 0.001; β = 0.20, p < 0.05, respectively); (2) Family SES significantly predicted the initial levels of oral vocabulary knowledge (β = 0.35, p < 0.001); (3) Growth rate in oral vocabulary knowledge was predicted significantly by family SES (β = 0.26, p < 0.05) and children’s homograph awareness (β = 0.30, p < 0.01), respectively. The present findings have an important role in elucidating developmental changes of children’s oral vocabulary knowledge. It also highlights the predictive role of family SES, phonological awareness, homograph awareness, and compounding awareness in oral vocabulary development. More importantly, in the consideration of our findings that family SES and homograph awareness at Time 1 could predict growth rate in children’s oral vocabulary knowledge from T1 to T5. To improve the oral vocabulary knowledge for children, teachers need to pay special attention to children’s homograph awareness.
Key wordsoral vocabulary knowledge    developmental trajectories    latent growth modeling    family SES    morphological awareness
     出版日期: 2017-12-26
ZTFLH:     
  B844  
基金资助: 浙江省哲学社会科学规划课题(17NDJC120YB); 教育部人文社会科学研究规划青年基金项目(16YJC190002); 国家社会科学基金重大项目(13&ZD188)。
通讯作者: 伍新春, E-mail: xcwu@bnu.edu.cn   
引用本文:   
程亚华, 伍新春, 刘红云, 李虹.  小学低年级儿童口语词汇知识的发展轨迹及其影响因素[J]. 心理学报, 2018, 50(2): 206-215.
CHENG Yahua, WU Xinchun, LIU Hongyun, LI Hong.  The developmental trajectories of oral vocabulary knowledge and its influential factors in Chinese primary school students. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2018, 50(2): 206-215.
链接本文:  
http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/10.3724/SP.J.1041.2018.00206      或      http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlxb/CN/Y2018/V50/I2/206
[1] 顾红磊, 刘君, 夏天生.  家庭社会经济地位对小学生阅读自主性的影响: 父母鼓励和阅读动机的中介作用[J]. 心理学报, 2017, 49(8): 1063-1071.
[2] 张杉杉;陈楠;刘红云. LGM模型中缺失数据处理方法的比较: ML方法与Diggle-Kenward选择模型[J]. 心理学报, 2017, 49(5): 699-710.
[3] 侯金芹;陈祉妍. 青少年抑郁情绪的发展轨迹:界定亚群组及其影响因素[J]. 心理学报, 2016, 48(8): 957-968.
[4] 赵英;程亚华;伍新春;阮氏芳. 汉语儿童语素意识与词汇知识的双向关系:一项追踪研究[J]. 心理学报, 2016, 48(11): 1434-1444.
[5] 李利平; 伍新春;周宁宁;程亚华; 阮氏芳. 汉语儿童读词者的认知特征及其影响因素[J]. 心理学报, 2016, 48(10): 1270-1281.
[6] 刘俊升;周颖;李丹. 童年中晚期孤独感的发展轨迹:一项潜变量增长模型分析[J]. 心理学报, 2013, 45(2): 179-192.
[7] 张晓. 童年早期社会能力的发展:一个潜变量增长模型[J]. 心理学报, 2011, 43(12): 1388-1397.
[8] 陈亮,张文新,纪林芹,陈光辉,魏星,常淑敏. 童年中晚期攻击的发展轨迹和性别差异:基于母亲报告的分析[J]. 心理学报, 2011, 43(06): 629-638.
Viewed
Full text


Abstract

Cited

  Shared   
  Discussed   
版权所有 © 《心理学报》编辑部
本系统由北京玛格泰克科技发展有限公司设计开发  技术支持:support@magtech.com.cn