Please wait a minute...
Advances in Psychological Science    2019, Vol. 27 Issue (8) : 1468-1477     DOI: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2019.01468
Regular Articles |
Intuition-based prosociality: Thinking based on social heuristics hypothesis
SHI Rong,LIU Chang()
School of Psychology, Nanjing Normal University, Nanjing 210097, China.
Download: PDF(620 KB)   HTML
Export: BibTeX | EndNote | Reference Manager | ProCite | RefWorks     Supporting Info
Guide   
Abstract  

Prosociality is a central feature of human beings and a major focus of research across the natural and social sciences. Most theoretical models of prosociality share a common assumption: Humans are instinctively selfish, and prosocial behaviors require exerting reflective control over these basic instincts. However, the intuitive model of prosociality has recently contradicted this view. This model assumes that the understanding of prosociality should be revised to include the possibility that, in many cases, prosocial behavior -- instead of requiring active control over our impulses -- represents an impulse of its own. The intuitive model of prosociality is supported by at least three markers: (1) behavioral signs of automaticity, (2) neural signatures of reward seeking, and (3) early development. Furthermore, based on social heuristics hypothesis, we explained human’s intuitive prosociality and analyzed the context and the individual factors which affect human’s intuitive prosocial behavior. These observations provided a new perspective for researchers and challenged them to reconsider the roots of prosociality.

Keywords Intuition      prosocial behavior      intuitive model of prosociality      social heuristics hypothesis     
ZTFLH:  B849: C91  
Corresponding Authors: Chang LIU     E-mail: liuchang@njnu.edu.cn
Issue Date: 25 June 2019
Service
E-mail this article
E-mail Alert
RSS
Articles by authors
Rong SHI
Chang LIU
Cite this article:   
Rong SHI,Chang LIU. Intuition-based prosociality: Thinking based on social heuristics hypothesis[J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2019, 27(8): 1468-1477.
URL:  
http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlkxjz/EN/10.3724/SP.J.1042.2019.01468     OR     http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlkxjz/EN/Y2019/V27/I8/1468
[1] 费定舟, 钱东海, 黄旭辰 . (2016). 利他行为的自我控制过程模型: 自我损耗下的道德情绪的正向作用. 心理学报, 48( 9), 1175-1183.
[2] 任俊, 李瑞雪, 詹鋆, 刘迪, 林曼, 彭年强 . (2014). 好人可能做出坏行为的心理学解释——基于自我控制资源损耗的研究证据. 心理学报, 46( 6), 841-851.
[3] 人民网 . (2017. 2016年我国接收国内外款物捐赠1392.94亿元. 2017-11-03取自
url: http://society.people.com.cn/n1/2017/1103/c1008-29624235.html
[4] 张和云 . (2016). 善良人格的结构、认知加工特点及其对善行表达的影响研究 (博士学位论文). 北京师范大学.
[5] 张振, 张帆, 黄亮, 袁博, 王益文 . (2014). 决策中社会价值取向的理论与测评方法. 心理科学进展, 22( 1), 48-56.
[6] Aknin L. B., Dunn E. W., & Norton M. I . (2012). Happiness runs in a circular motion: Evidence for a positive feedback loop between prosocial spending and happiness. Journal of Happiness Studies, 13( 2), 347-355.
[7] Batson C. D., & Adam A. P ., (2003). Altruism and prosocial behavior. In T. Millon & M. J. Lerner (Eds.), I. B. Weiner (Ed.-in-chief). . Handbook of psychology: Personality and social psychology. (Vol.5, pp.463-484 ). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley & Sons.
[8] Bear A. &Rand D.G . (2016). Intuition, deliberation, and the evolution of cooperation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 113( 4), 936-941.
[9] Bolton G.., &Ockenfels A., (2000). ERC: A theory of equity, reciprocity, and competition. American Economic Review, 90( 1), 166-193.
[10] Burger J. M., Messian N., Patel S., del Prado A., & Anderson C . (2004). What a coincidence! The effects of incidental similarity on compliance. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30( 1), 35-43.
[11] Capraro V. &Cococcioni G., (2015). Social setting, intuition and experience in laboratory experiments interact to shape cooperative decision-making. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 282( 1811), 1003-1007.
[12] Capraro V. &Cococcioni G., (2016). Rethinking spontaneous giving: Extreme time pressure and ego-depletion favor self-regarding reactions. Scientific Reports, 6, Article 27219.
[13] Carlson R. W., Aknin L. B., & Liotti M . (2016). When is giving an impulse? An ERP investigation of intuitive prosocial behavior. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 11( 7), 1121-1129.
[14] Chudek M. &Henrich J., (2011). Culture-gene co-evolution, norm-psychology and the emergence of human prosociality. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15( 5), 218-226.
[15] Cohn J., Squire P., Estabrooke I. , & O’Neill E. (2013) .Enhancing intuitive decision making through implicit learning. In: Schmorrow D. D., Fidopiastis C. M. (Eds) Foundations of Augmented Cognition. AC 2013. Lecture Notes in Computer Science, vol 8027. Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg.
[16] Cornelissen G., Dewitte S., & Warlop L . (2011). Are social value orientations expressed automatically? Decision making in the dictator game. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 37( 8), 1080-1090.
[17] Cox A., Kohls G., Naples A. J., Mukerji C. E., Coffman M. C., Rutherford H. J., .. McPartland J. C . (2015). Diminished social reward anticipation in the broad autism phenotype as revealed by event-related brain potentials. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 10( 10), 1357-1364.
[18] Cropanzano R., Byrne Z. S., Bobocel D. R., & Rupp D. E . (2001). Moral virtues, fairness heuristics, social entities, and other denizens of organizational justice. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 58( 2), 164-209.
[19] Dawes C. T., Loewen P. J., Schreiber D., Simmons A. N., Flagan T., McElreath R., .. Paulus M. P . (2012). Neural basis of egalitarian behavior. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109, 6479-6483.
[20] DeSteno D. (2009). Social emotions and intertemporal choice, “hot” mechanisms for building social and economic capital. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 18( 5), 280-284.
[21] DeVoe S.., &House J., (2016). Replications with MTurkers who are na?ve versus experienced with academic studies: A comment on connors, khamitov, moroz, campbell, and henderson (2015). Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 67, 65-67.
[22] D?ssing F., Piovesan M., & Wengstr?m E . (2017). Cognitive load and cooperation. Review of Behavioral Economics, 4( 1), 69-81.
[23] Dunham Y., Baron A. S., & Banaji M. R . (2008). The development of implicit intergroup cognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12( 7), 248-253.
[24] Ellingsen T., Herrmann B., Nowak M., Rand D., & Tarnita C. (2012). Civic capital in two cultures: The nature of cooperation in Romania and USA. Working paper, Stockholm School of Economics, Stockholm. .
url: http://ssrn.com/abstractD2179575
[25] Engel C. &Rand D.G . (2014). What does “clean” really mean? The implicit framing of decontextualized experiments. Economics Letters, 122( 3), 386-389.
[26] Evans J. S. . (2017). Dual-process theories.In L. J. Ball, & V. A. Thompson (Eds.). International Handbook of Thinking and Reasoning(p. 173-188). New York, NY: Routledge.
[27] Feygina I. &Henry P.J . (2015). Culture and prosocial behavior.In D. A. Schroeder,& W. G.Eds.). The Oxford handbook of prosocial behavior
[28] Foerde K., Knowlton B. J., & Poldrack R. A . (2006). Modulation of competing memory systems by distraction. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 103( 31), 11778-11783.
[29] Garon N., Bryson S. E., & Smith I. M . (2008). Executive function in preschoolers: A review using an integrative framework. Psychological Bulletin, 134( 1), 31-60.
[30] G?rtner M. (2018). The prosociality of intuitive decisions depends on the status quo. Journal of Behavioral and Experimental Economics, 74, 127-138.
[31] Halali E., Bereby-Meyer Y., & Meiran N . (2014). Between self-interest and reciprocity: The social bright side of self-control failure. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 143( 2), 745-754.
[32] Hamlin J. K., Wynn K., & Bloom P . (2007). Social evaluation by preverbal infants. Nature,450(7169), 557-559.
[33] Hare T. A., Camerer C. F., & Rangel A . (2009). Self-control in decision-making involves modulation of the vmPFC valuation system. Science, 324( 5927), 646-648.
[34] Heilman M.., &Chen J.J . (2005). Same behavior, different consequences: Reactions to men’s and women’s altruistic citizenship behavior. Journal of Applied Psychology, 90( 3), 431-441.
[35] Henrich J., Boyd R., Bowles S., Camerer C., Fehr E., Gintis H., .. Tracer D . (2005). “Economic man” in cross-cultural perspective: Behavioral experiments in 15 small-scale societies. Behavioral and Brain Sciences,28(6), 795-855.
[36] Henrich J., Heine S. J., & Norenzayan A . (2010). The weirdest people in the world?. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 33( 2-3), 61-83.
[37] Kahneman D. (2011). Thinking, fast and slow.New York, NY: Farrar, Straus & Giroux.
[38] Knowlton B. J., Mangels J. A., & Squire L. R . (1996). A neostriatal habit learning system in humans. Science, 273( 5280), 1399-1402.
[39] Krupka E.., &Weber R.A . (2013). Identifying social norms using coordination games: Why does dictator game sharing vary? Journal of the European Economic Association, 11( 3), 495-524.
[40] Lepper M. R., Greene D., & Nisbett R. E . (1973). Undermining children’s intrinsic interest with extrinsic reward: A test of the “overjustification” hypothesis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 28( 1), 129-137.
[41] Lieberman M. D. (2007).The X- and C-systems:The neural basis of automatic and controlled social cognition. In E. HarmonJones & P. Winkielman (Eds.), Fundamentals of social neuroscience (pp. 290-315) New York, NY: Guilford Press.
[42] Liu C-J. &Hao F., (2011). An application of a dual-process approach to decision making in social dilemmas. American Journal of Psychology, 124( 2), 203-212.
[43] Lotz S. (2015). Spontaneous giving under structural inequality: Intuition promotes cooperation in asymmetric social dilemmas. PLoS One, 10( 7), e0131562.
[44] Ma Y., Liu Y., Rand D. G., Heatherton T. F., & Han S . (2015). Opposing oxytocin effects on intergroup cooperative behavior in intuitive and reflective minds. Neuropsychopharmacology, 40( 10), 2379-2387.
[45] McAuliffe K., Blake P. R., Steinbeis N., & Warneken F . (2017). The developmental foundations of human fairness. Nature Human Behaviour, 1( 2), 1-9.
[46] Mischkowski D., Thielmann I., & Gl?ckner A . (2018). Think it through before making a choice? Processing mode does not influence social mindfulness. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 74, 85-97.
[47] Moll J., Krueger F., Zahn R., Pardini M., de Oliveira-Souza R., & Grafman J . (2006). Human fronto-mesolimbic networks guide decisions about charitable donation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences,103(42), 15623-15628.
[48] Morelli S. A., Rameson L.T., Lieberman M. D . (2014). The neuralcomponents of empathy: Predicting daily prosocial behavior. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience,9(1), 39-47.
[49] Niendam T. A., Laird A. R., Ray K. L., Dean Y. M., Glahn D. C., & Carter C. S . (2012). Meta-analytic evidence for a superordinate cognitive control network subserving diverse executive functions. Cognitive,Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 12(2), 241-268.
[50] Penner L. A., Dovidio J. F., Piliavin J. A., & Schroeder D. A . (2005). Prosocial behavior: multilevel perspectives. Annual Review of Psychology,56 365-392.
[51] Peysakhovich A. &Rand D.G . (2015). Habits of virtue: Creating norms of cooperation and defection in the laboratory. Management Science,62(3), 631-647.
[52] Pfabigan D. M., Seidel E-M., Sladky R., Hahn A., Paul K., Grahl A., .. Windischberger C . (2014). P300 amplitude variation is related to ventral striatum BOLD response during gain and loss anticipation: An EEG and fMRI experiment. NeuroImage,96(100), 12-21.
[53] Protzko J., Ouimette B., & Schooler J . (2016). Believing there is no free will corrupts intuitive cooperation. Cognition,151 6-9.
[54] Rand D. . (2017). Social dilemma cooperation (unlike Dictator Game giving) is intuitive for men as well as women. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology,73 164-168.
[55] Rand D. G., Brescoll V. L., Everett J. A. C., Capraro V., & Barcelo H . (2016). Social heuristics and social roles: Intuition favors altruism for women but not for men. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General,145(4), 389-396.
[56] Rand D.., &Epstein Z.G . (2014). Risking your life without a second thought: Intuitive decision-making and extreme altruism. PLoS One,9(10), e109687.
[57] Rand D. G., Greene J. D., & Nowak M. A . (2012). Spontaneous giving and calculated greed. Nature,489(7416), 427-430.
[58] Rand D.., &Kraft-Todd G.T . (2014). Reflection does not undermine self-interested prosociality. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience,8 300.
[59] Rand D.., &Nowak M.A . (2013). Human cooperation. Trends in Cognitive Sciences,17(8), 413-425.
[60] Rand D. G., Newman G. E., & Wurzbacher O. M . (2015). Social context and the dynamics of cooperative choice. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making,28(2), 159-166.
[61] Rand D. G., Peysakhovich A., Kraft-Todd G. T., Newman G. E., Wurzbacher O., Nowak M. A., & Greene J. D . (2014). Social heuristics shape intuitive cooperation. Nature Communications,5(3677), 1-12.
[62] Righetti F., Finkenauer C., & Finkel E. J . (2013). Low self-control promotes the willingness to sacrifice in close relationships. Psychological Science,24(8), 1533-1540.
[63] Ruff C.., &Fehr E., (2014). The neurobiology of rewards and values in social decision making. Nature Reviews Neuroscience,15(8), 549-562.
[64] Shenhav A., Rand D. G., & Greene J. D . (2012). Divine intuition: Cognitive style influences belief in God. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General,141(3), 423-428.
[65] Steinbeis N., Bernhardt B. C., & Singer T . (2012). Impulse control and underlying functions of the left DLPFC mediate age-related and age-independent individual differences in strategic social behavior. Neuron,73(5), 1040-1051.
[66] Stevens J.., &Hauser M.D . (2004). Why be nice? Psychological constraints on the evolution of cooperation. Trends in Cognitive Sciences,8(2), 60-65.
[67] Stivala A., Kashima Y., & Kirley M . (2016). Culture and cooperation in a spatial public goods game . Physical Review E,94(3-1), 032303.
[68] Swami V., Voracek M., Stieger S., Tran U. S., & Furnham A . (2014). Analytic thinking reduces belief in conspiracy theories. Cognition,133(3), 572-585.
[69] Tingh?g G., Andersson D., Bonn C., B?ttiger H., Josephson C., Lundgren G., .. Johannesson M . (2013). Intuition and cooperation reconsidered. Nature,498(7452), E1-E2.
[70] Tricomi E., Rangel A., Camerer C. F., & O’Doherty J. P . (2010). Neural evidence for inequality-averse social preferences. Nature,463(7284), 1089-1091.
[71] Warneken F. (2015). Precocious prosociality: Why do young children help? Child Development Perspectives,9(1), 1-6.
[72] Warneken F. &Tomasello M., (2009). Varieties of altruism in children and chimpanzees. Trends in Cognitive Sciences,13(9), 397-402.
[73] Zaki J. A., &Mitchell J. P (2016). Prosociality as a form of reward-seeking. In J. D. Greene, I. Morrison, & M. E. P. Seligman (Eds.), Positive neuroscience (pp. 57-72). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
[74] Zaki J. &Mitchell J.P . (2013). Intuitive prosociality. Current Directions in Psychological Science,22(6), 466-470.
[75] Zitek E. M., Jordan A. H., Monin B., & Leach F. R . (2010). Victim entitlement to behave selfishly. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,98(2), 245-255.
[1] TANG Ming,LI Weiqiang,LIU Fuhui,YUAN Bo. The association between guilt and prosocial behavior: A systematic review and meta-analysis[J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2019, 27(5): 773-788.
[2] CHEN Qingwei, RU Taotao, ZHOU Juyan, LI Jinghua, XIONG Xiao, LI Xiaoran, ZHOU Guofu. The effects of light on social cognition and social behavior[J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2018, 26(6): 1083-1095.
[3] ZOU Xiaoyan, YIN Keli, LU Lin.  Collective rituals promote cohesion: Based on action, emotion, and memory[J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2018, 26(5): 939-950.
[4] CHENG Nanhua, LI Zhanxing, ZHU Liqi.  Children’s understanding of social power and its relationship with social behavior[J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2018, 26(2): 283-293.
[5] ZHANG Xuejiao; LIU Conghui. The watching eyes effect on prosociality[J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2017, 25(3): 475-485.
[6] DING Fengqin; LU Zhaohui. Association between empathy and prosocial behavior: A systematic review and meta-analysis[J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2016, 24(8): 1159-1174.
[7] YUAN Mingliang; ZHANG Mengyuan; KOU Yu. Prosocial reputation and prosocial behavior[J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2016, 24(10): 1655-1662.
[8] YE Hongyan; ZHANG Fenghua. Embodiment in Moral Judgment[J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2015, 23(8): 1480-1488.
[9] YANG Ying; KOU Yu. Individuals’ Well-Being in Prosocial Interaction: The Role of Autonomy[J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2015, 23(7): 1226-1235.
[10] DONG Mengchen; WU Song; ZHU Yijie; GUO Yafei; JIN Shenghua. Religion and Prosocial Behavior[J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2015, 23(6): 1095-1108.
[11] TANG Jiangwei; LU Hong; LIU Yi; PENG Jian. Exploring Moral Intuitive Decision-making and Its Mechanism[J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2015, 23(10): 1830-1842.
[12] WANG-Peng;FANG-Ping;JIANG-Yuan. Moral Decision-Making in the Context of Moral Intuition: Exploring the Influcence Factors[J]. , 2011, 19(4): 573-579.
[13] TIAN Xue-Hong;YANG Qun;ZHANG De-Xuan;ZHANG Ye. A Theory on the Mechanism Underlying Moral Intuitions[J]. , 2011, 19(10): 1426-1433.
[14] YU Cheng-Fu;ZHANG Wei;LI Dong-Ping;XIAO Jie-Ting. Gratitude and Its Relationship with Well-Being[J]. , 2010, 18(07): 1110-1121.
[15] XIE Xi-Yao; LUO Yue-Jia. The Role of Emotion in Moral Judgment: Evidence from Cognitive Neuroscience[J]. , 2009, 17(06): 1250-1256.
Viewed
Full text


Abstract

Cited

  Shared   
  Discussed   
Copyright © Advances in Psychological Science
Support by Beijing Magtech