Please wait a minute...
Advances in Psychological Science    2018, Vol. 26 Issue (6) : 1012-1018     DOI: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2018.01012
Regular Articles |
Implicit acquisition of musical syntax and its influence on schema expectation
Jue DENG1,Yiduo YE1(),Yanfang CHEN2
1 School of Psychology, Fujian Normal University, Fuzhou 350000, China
2 Art College, Xiamen University, Xiamen 361005, China
Download: PDF(324 KB)   HTML
Export: BibTeX | EndNote | Reference Manager | ProCite | RefWorks     Supporting Info
Guide   
Abstract  

The acquisition of musical syntax derives from the long-term abstraction of statistical rules of musical schema, which depends on the daily music exposure and statistical learning ability. It plays an important role in the process of constructing musical mental lexicon and influences the formation of musical schema expectation. The musical syntax has been proved to be implicitly learned with unconscious exposure, while explicit professional training may be conducive to the improvement of music statistical learning ability. The researches of the ENAN, N5, mCPS and other musical syntax-related electroencephalograms help to understand the processing mechanism of implicit learning of musical syntax. Therefore, future studies should examine, in the complete music structure, how the acquisitions of various music elements interact to make music learning experiments more ecologically effective and to promote the application of the research results in music teaching practice.

Keywords implicit acquisition      musical syntax      statistical learning      exposure effect      musical expectation     
ZTFLH:  B842  
Fund: 
Corresponding Authors: Yiduo YE     E-mail: yeyiduo@163.com
Issue Date: 28 April 2018
Service
E-mail this article
E-mail Alert
RSS
Articles by authors
Jue DENG
Yiduo YE
Yanfang CHEN
Cite this article:   
Jue DENG,Yiduo YE,Yanfang CHEN. Implicit acquisition of musical syntax and its influence on schema expectation[J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2018, 26(6): 1012-1018.
URL:  
http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlkxjz/EN/10.3724/SP.J.1042.2018.01012     OR     http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlkxjz/EN/Y2018/V26/I6/1012
[1] 蒋存梅 . (2016). 音乐心理学. 上海: 华东师范大学出版社.
[2] 李菲菲, 宋晨璐, 刘宝根 . ( 2017). 轻中度智力障碍儿童的视觉统计学习. 中国特殊教育, (8), 44-49.
url: http://d.wanfangdata.com.cn/Periodical/zgtsjy201708015
[3] 罗小平, 黄虹 . (2008). 音乐心理学 (第2版). 上海: 上海音乐学院出版社.
[4] 马谐, 杨玉芳, 张秋月 . ( 2016). 音乐句法的加工. 科学通报, 61( 10), 1099-1111.
[5] 马谐, 杨玉芳 . ( 2017). 音乐句法嵌套结构加工的认知神经机制. 中国心理学会音乐心理学专业委员会(筹)成立大会暨首届学术年会, 上海.
[6] 欧阳玥, 戴志强 . ( 2010). 音乐节拍认知的研究评述. 心理科学进展, 18( 11), 1692-1699.
url: http://d.wanfangdata.com.cn/Periodical/xlxdt201011003
[7] 张晶晶, 杨玉芳 . ( 2017). 音乐句法加工的影响因素. 心理科学进展, 25( 11), 1823-1830.
url: http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlkxjz/CN/article/article4051.shtml
[8] 张前, 王次炤 . (1992). 音乐美学基础. 北京: 人民音乐出版社.
[9] 郑茂平 . ( 2006). 音乐知觉期待的研究述评. 心理科学进展, 14( 6), 866-872.
url: http://d.wanfangdata.com.cn/Periodical/xlxdt200606011
[10] 郑茂平 . ( 2010). 音乐审美期待的心理实质——迈尔期待理论及相关延展研究的心理学解释. 中央音乐学院学报, (3), 126-136.
url: http://www.cnki.com.cn/Article/CJFDTotal-ZYYE201003016.htm
[11] Batterink L. J., Reber P. J., Neville H. J., & Paller K. A . ( 2015). Implicit and explicit contributions to statistical learning. Journal of Memory & Language, 83, 62-78.
pmid: 4448134 url: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/26034344
[12] Besson, M., & Fa?ta, F. (1995). An event-related potential (ERP) study of musical expectancy: Comparison of musicians with nonmusicians. Journal of Experimental Psychology Human Perception & Performance, 21( 6), 1278-1296.
url: http://psycnet.apa.org/journals/xhp/21/6/1278/
[13] Bharucha, J. J., & Stoeckig, K. (1987). Priming of chords: Spreading activation or overlapping frequency spectra? Perception & Psychophysics, 41( 6), 519-524.
pmid: 3615148 url: http://psycnet.apa.org/record/1988-25420-001
[14] Bly B. M., Carrión R. E., & Rasch B . ( 2009). Domain- specific learning of grammatical structure in musical and phonological sequences. Memory & Cognition, 37( 1), 10-20.
pmid: 19103971 url: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/19103971
[15] Brochard R., Abecasis D., Potter D., Ragot R., & Drake C . ( 2003). The "Ticktock" of our internal clock: Direct brain evidence of subjective accents in isochronous sequences. Psychological Science, 14( 4), 362-366.
url: http://journals.sagepub.com/doi/10.1111/1467-9280.24441
[16] Dienes, Z., & Longuet-Higgins, C. (2004). Can musical transformations be implicitly learned?. Cognitive Science, 28( 4), 531-558.
url: http://doi.wiley.com/10.1207/s15516709cog2804_2
[17] Egermann H., Pearce M. T., Wiggins G. A., & McAdams S . ( 2013). Probabilistic models of expectation violation predict psychophysiological emotional responses to live concert music. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 13( 3), 533-553.
pmid: 23605956 url: http://europepmc.org/abstract/MED/23605956
[18] Ettlinger M., Margulis E. H.,& Wong, P. C. M. (2011). Implicit memory in music and language. Frontiers in Psychology, 2( 6), 211.
pmid: 21927608 url: http://pubmedcentralcanada.ca/pmcc/articles/PMC3170172/
[19] Frost R., Armstrong B. C., Siegelman N., & Christiansen M. H . ( 2015). Domain generality vs. modality specificity: The paradox of statistical learning. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 19( 3), 117-125.
pmid: 25631249 url: http://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S1364661314002770
[20] Grahn, J. A., & Schuit, D. (2012). Individual differences in rhythmic ability: Behavioral and neuroimaging investigations. Psychomusicology, 22( 2), 105-121.
url: http://doi.apa.org/getdoi.cfm?doi=10.1037/a0031188
[21] Guo, S., & Koelsch, S. (2016). Effects of veridical expectations on syntax processing in music: Event-related potential evidence. Scientific Reports, 6, 19064.
pmid: 4726113 url: http://www.nature.com/articles/srep19064
[22] Halpern A. R., Zioga I., Shankleman M., Lindsen J., Pearce M. T., & Bhattacharya J . ( 2017). That note sounds wrong! Age-related effects in processing of musical expectation. Brain & Cognition, 113, 1-9.
pmid: 28064077 url: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/28064077
[23] Hannon, E. E., & Trainor, L. J . ( 2007). Music acquisition: Effects of enculturation and formal training on development. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11( 11), 466-472.
pmid: 17981074 url: http://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S1364661307002410
[24] Hodges D. A. (2006). Handbook of music psychology. San Antonio: IMR Press. (Original work published 1996)
[25] [ 霍杰斯 , D. A. (2006). 音乐心理学手册 (刘沛, 任恺译). 长沙: 湖南文艺出版社.]
[26] Honing H., Ladinig O., Háden G. P., & Winkler I . ( 2009). Is beat induction innate or learned?. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1169, 93-96.
url: http://doi.wiley.com/10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.04761.x
[27] Ignacio, D. A . ( 2016). The cognitive effects of musical expectations on affective priming. Psychomusicology: Music, Mind, & Brain, 26( 4), 358-365.
url: http://psycnet.apa.org/journals/pmu/26/4/358/
[28] James C. E., Cereghetti D. M., Tribes E. R., & Oechslin M. S . ( 2015). Electrophysiological evidence for a specific neural correlate of musical violation expectation in primary-school children. NeuroImage, 104, 386-397.
pmid: 25278251 url: http://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S1053811914007897
[29] Jentschke S., Koelsch S., Sallat S., & Friederici A. D . ( 2008). Children with specific language impairment also show impairment of music-syntactic processing. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20( 11), 1940-1951.
pmid: 18416683 url: http://www.mitpressjournals.org/doi/10.1162/jocn.2008.20135
[30] Jentschke S., Friederici A. D., & Koelsch S . ( 2014). Neural correlates of music-syntactic processing in two-year old children. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 9, 200-208.
pmid: 24907450 url: http://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S1878929314000322
[31] Jonaitis, E. M., & Saffran, J. R . ( 2009). Learning harmony: The role of serial statistics. Cognitive Science, 33( 5), 951-968.
pmid: 21585492 url: http://doi.wiley.com/10.1111/j.1551-6709.2009.01036.x
[32] Jongsma M. L. A., Desain P., & Honing H . ( 2004). Rhythmic context influences the auditory evoked potentials of musicians and nonmusicians. Biological Psychology, 66( 2), 129-152.
pmid: 15041136 url: http://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0301051103001662
[33] Kn?sche T. R., Neuhaus C., Haueisen J., Alter K., Maess B., Witte O. W., & Friederici A. D . ( 2005). Perception of phrase structure in music. Human Brain Mapping, 24( 4), 259-273.
pmid: 15678484 url: http://doi.wiley.com/10.1002/hbm.v24%3A4
[34] Koelsch S., Gunter T., Friederici A. D., & Schr?ger E . ( 2000). Brain indices of music processing: "nonmusicians" are musical. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12( 3), 520-541.
url: http://www.mitpressjournals.org/doi/10.1162/089892900562183
[35] Koelsch S., Gunter T. C., Wittfoth M., & Sammler D . ( 2005). Interaction between syntax processing in language and in music: An ERP study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17( 10), 1565-1577.
pmid: 16269097 url: http://www.mitpressjournals.org/doi/10.1162/089892905774597290
[36] Koelsch, S., & Sammler, D. (2008). Cognitive components of regularity processing in the auditory domain. PLoS One, 3( 7), e2650.
pmid: 2440538 url: http://dx.plos.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0002650
[37] Kuhn, G., & Dienes, Z. (2006). Differences in the types of musical regularity learnt in incidental- and intentional- learning conditions. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 59( 10), 1725-1744.
pmid: 16945857 url: http://journals.sagepub.com/doi/10.1080/17470210500438361
[38] Leman, M. (2000). An auditory model of the role of short-term memory in probe-tone ratings. Music Perception: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 17( 4), 481-509.
url: http://mp.ucpress.edu/cgi/doi/10.2307/40285830
[39] Loui, P. (2012). Learning and liking of melody and harmony: Further studies in artificial grammar learning. Topics in Cognitive Science, 4( 4), 554-567.
pmid: 3465524 url: http://doi.wiley.com/10.1111/tops.2012.4.issue-4
[40] Loui, P., & Wessel, D. (2008). Learning and liking an artificial musical system: Effects of set size and repeated exposure. Musicae Scientiae the Journal of the European Society for the Cognitive Sciences of Music, 12( 2), 207-230.
pmid: 20151034 url: http://pubmedcentralcanada.ca/pmcc/articles/PMC2819428/
[41] Loui P., Wessel D. L.,& Hudson Kam, C. L. (2010). Humans rapidly learn grammatical structure in a new musical scale. Music Perception, 27( 5), 377-388.
pmid: 2927013 url: http://mp.ucpress.edu/cgi/doi/10.1525/mp.2010.27.5.377
[42] Loui P., Wu E. H., Wessel D. L., & Knight R. T . ( 2009). A generalized mechanism for perception of pitch patterns. Journal of Neuroscience, 29( 2), 454-459.
pmid: 19144845 url: http://www.jneurosci.org/cgi/doi/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4503-08.2009
[43] Nan Y., Kn?sche T. R., & Friederici A. D . ( 2009). Non-musicians' perception of phrase boundaries in music: A cross-cultural ERP study. Biological Psychology, 82( 1), 70-81.
pmid: 19540302 url: http://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0301051109001240
[44] Patel A. D. (2012). Music, language, and the brain. New York: Oxford University Press. (Original work published 2008)
[45] [ 帕特尔 , A. D. (2012). 音乐、语言与脑 (杨玉芳, 蔡丹超等译). 上海: 华东师范大学出版社.]
[46] Pearce, M. T., & Christensen, J. F . ( 2012). Conference report: The neurosciences and music-IV—learning and memory. Psychomusicology, 22( 1), 70-73.
url: http://doi.apa.org/getdoi.cfm?doi=10.1037/a0027235
[47] Peretz, I. (1996). Can we lose memory for music? A case of music agnosia in a nonmusician. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 8( 6), 481-496.
pmid: 23961980 url: http://www.mitpressjournals.org/doi/10.1162/jocn.1996.8.6.481
[48] Rohrmeier, M. A., & Cross, I. (2010). Narmour’s principles affect implicit learning of melody. In S. M. Demorest (Ed.), Proceedings of the 11th international conference on music perception and cognition (ICMPC 2010)(pp. 1-5). Seattle, WA: University of Washington School of Music.
url: http://www.mendeley.com/research/narmours-principles-affect-implicit-learning-melody/
[49] Rohrmeier, M., & Rebuschat, P. (2012). Implicit learning and acquisition of music. Topics in Cognitive Science, 4( 4), 525-553.
pmid: 23060126 url: http://doi.wiley.com/10.1111/tops.2012.4.issue-4
[50] Rohrmeier M., Rebuschat P., & Cross I . ( 2011). Incidental and online learning of melodic structure. Consciousness & Cognition, 20( 2), 214-222.
pmid: 20832338 url: http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S1053810010001467
[51] Schubert E.,& Pearce, M.( 2015). A new look at musical expectancy: The veridical versus the general in the mental organization of music. In International symposium on computer music multidisciplinary research (pp. 358-370). Springer International Publishing.
url: http://link.springer.com/chapter/10.1007%2F978-3-319-46282-0_23
[52] Schultz B. G., Stevens C. J., Keller P. E., & Tillmann B . ( 2013). The implicit learning of metrical and nonmetrical temporal patterns. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 66( 2), 360-380.
pmid: 22943558 url: http://journals.sagepub.com/doi/10.1080/17470218.2012.712146
[53] Seger C. A., Spiering B. J., Sares A. G., Quraini S. I., Alpeter C., David J., & Thaut M. H . ( 2013). Corticostriatal contributions to musical expectancy perception. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 25( 7), 1062-1077.
pmid: 23410032 url: http://www.mitpressjournals.org/doi/10.1162/jocn_a_00371
[54] Szpunar K. K., Schellenberg E. G., & Pliner P . ( 2004). Liking and memory for musical stimuli as a function of exposure. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 30( 2), 370-381.
pmid: 14979811 url: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/14979811?dopt=AbstractPlus
[55] Thompson W. F., Balkwill L. L., & Vernescu R . ( 2000). Expectancies generated by recent exposure to melodic sequences. Memory & Cognition, 28( 4), 547-555.
pmid: 10946538 url: http://link.springer.com/article/10.3758/BF03201245
[56] Tillmann B., Stevens C., & Keller P. E . ( 2011). Learning of timing patterns and the development of temporal expectations. Psychological Research, 75( 3), 243-258.
pmid: 20683612 url: http://link.springer.com/10.1007/s00426-010-0302-7
[57] Tillmann B., Gosselin N., Bigand E., & Peretz I . ( 2012). Priming paradigm reveals harmonic structure processing in congenital amusia. Cortex, 48( 8), 1073-1078.
pmid: 22326325 url: http://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0010945212000068
[58] Trainor, L. J., & Corrigall, K. A . ( 2010). Music acquisition and effects of musical experience. In J. M. Riess, R. Fay, & A. Popper (Eds.), Music perception (Vol. 36, pp. 89-127). New York, NY: Springer.
url: http://dx.doi.org/ acquisition and effects of musical experience. In J. M. Riess, R. Fay,
[59] Zajonc, R. B . ( 2001). Mere exposure: A gateway to the subliminal. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 10( 6), 224-228.
url: http://journals.sagepub.com/doi/10.1111/1467-8721.00154
[60] Zhang J. J., Jiang C. M., Zhou L. S., & Yang Y. F . ( 2016). Perception of hierarchical boundaries in music and its modulation by expertise. Neuropsychologia, 91, 490-498.
pmid: 27659874 url: http://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0028393216303505
[1] Rui Wang; Yuan Shen; Peter Tino; Andrew Welchman; Zoe Kourtzi. Cortico-striatal mechanisms for learning predictive statistics in the human brain[J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2016, 24(Suppl.): 45-.
[2] WU Ying;YANG Yiyin;CHIU Chi-Yue. Exclusionary Reactions to Foreign Cultures in Globalization[J]. Advances in Psychological Science, 2014, 22(4): 721-730.
[3] SONG Xin-Yan;MENG Xiang-Zhi. Infants’ Speech Perception:The Development and Its Mechanisms[J]. , 2012, 20(6): 843-852.
[4]

Zhang Lirong;Guan Yijie;Wang Yong

. Theories of Mere Exposure Effect[J]. , 2006, 14(06): 932-937.
Viewed
Full text


Abstract

Cited

  Shared   
  Discussed   
Copyright © Advances in Psychological Science
Support by Beijing Magtech