[1] | 陈慧娟 . (2008). 论第一人称叙述视角的特性. 天津社会科学, 6, 110-112. | [2] | 崔刚 . (2015). 神经语言学. 北京: 清华大学出版社. | [3] | 申丹 . (1996). 论第一人称叙述与第三人称有限视角叙述在视角上的差异, 外国文学评论, 2, 14-23. | [4] | 申丹, 韩加明, 王丽亚 . (2018). 英美小说叙事理论研究. 北京: 北京大学出版社. | [5] | 亚里士多德 . (2002). 诗学 (罗念生译). 北京: 人民文学出版社. | [6] | 张曼, 刘欢欢 . (2018). 社会交流中人际神经同步的认知机制. 心理科学, 41(2), 378-383. | [7] | Altmann U., Bohrn I. C., Lubrich O., Menninghaus W., & Jacobs A. M . (2012). Fact vs fiction: How paratextual information shapes our reading processes, Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 9(1), 22-29. | [8] | Appel M., & Richter T . (2007). Persuasive effects of fictional narratives increase over time. Media Psychology, 10(1), 113-134. | [9] | Bal P. M., & Veltkamp M . (2013). How does fiction reading influence empathy? An experimental investigation on the role of emotional transportation. PLoS ONE, 8(1), e55341. | [10] | Baron-Cohen S., Wheelwright S., Hill J., Raste Y., & Plumb I . (2001). The “Reading the mind in the eyes” Test revised version: A study with normal adults, and adults with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42(2), 241-251. | [11] | Bowes A., & Katz A . (2015). Metaphor creates intimacy and temporarily enhances theory of mind. Memory & Cognition, 43(6), 953-963. | [12] | Brunyé T. T., Ditman T., Mahoney C. R., & Taylor H. A . (2011). Better you than I: Perspectives and emotion simulation during narrative comprehension. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 23(5), 659-666. | [13] | Buckner R. L., & Carroll D. C . (2007). Self-projection and the brain. Trends in Cognitive Science, 11(1), 49-57. | [14] | Burke M., Kuzmičová A., Mangen A., & Schilhab T . (2016). Empathy at the confluence of neuroscience and empirical literary studies. Scientific Study of Literature, 6(1), 6-41. | [15] | Dai H. Y., Chen L. J., Ni C. B., & Xu X. D . (2019). Literature reading modulates pronoun resolution in counterfactual world: Evidence from Event-related Potentials. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 45(5), 904-919. | [16] | de Mulder, H. N. M. Hakemulder F., van den Berghe R., Klaassen F., & van Berkum J. J. A . (2017). Effects of exposure to literary narrative fiction from book smart to street smart? Scientific Study of Literature, 7(1), 129-169. | [17] | Djikic M., Oatley K., & Moldoveanu M. C . (2013). Reading other minds: Effects of literature on empathy, Scientific Study of Literature, 3(1), 28-47. | [18] | Djikic M., & Oatley K . (2014). The art in fiction: From indirect communication to changes of the self. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 8(4), 498-505. | [19] | Dodell-Feder D., & Tamir D. I . (2018). Fiction reading has a small positive impact on social cognition: A meta-analysis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 147(11), 1713-1727. | [20] | Dvash J., & Shamay-Tsoory S. G . (2014). Theory of mind and empathy as multidimensional constructs: Neurological foundations. Topics in Language Disorders, 34(4), 282-295. | [21] | Fong K., Mullin J. B., & Mar R. A . (2013). What you read matters: The role of fiction genre in predicting interpersonal sensitivity. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 7(4), 370-376. | [22] | Hartung F., Burke M., Hagoort P., & Willems R. M . (2016). Taking perspective: Personal pronouns affect experiential aspects of literary reading. PLoS ONE, 11(5), e0154732. | [23] | Jacobs A. M., & Willems R. M . (2018). The fictive brain: Neurocognitive correlates of engagement in literature. Review of General Psychology, 22(2), 147-160. | [24] | Johnson D. R . (2013). Transportation into literary fiction reduces prejudice against, and increases empathy for Arab-Muslims. Scientific Study of Literature, 3(1), 77-92. | [25] | Kidd D. C., & Castano E . (2013). Reading literary fiction improves theory of mind. Science, 342(6156), 377-380. | [26] | Koopman E. M . (2018). Does originality evoke understanding? The relation between literary reading and empathy. Review of General Psychology, 22(2), 169-177. | [27] | Koopman E. M., & Hakemulder F . (2015). Effects of literature on empathy and self-reflection: A theoretical-empirical framework. Journal of Literary Theory, 9(1), 79-111. | [28] | Kotovych M., Peter D., Marisa B., & Mark H . (2011). Textual determinants of a component of literary identification. Scientific Study of Literature, 1(2), 260-291. | [29] | Małechi W., Pawłowski B., Cieński M., & Sorokowski P . (2018). Can fiction make us kinder to other species? The impact of fiction on pro-animal attitudes and behavior. Poetics, 66, 54-63. | [30] | Mar R. A . (2011). The neural bases of social cognition and story comprehension. Annual Review of Psychology, 62, 103-134. | [31] | Mar R., Oatley K., & Eng A . (2003). Abstraction and the vividness of details in fiction. Paper presented at the Annual Convention of the American Psychological Association, Toronto, Canada. | [32] | Mar R. A., Oatley K., Hirsh J., dela Paz J., & Peterson J. B . (2006). Bookworms versus nerds: Exposure to fiction versus non-fiction, divergent associations with social ability, and the simulation of fictional social worlds. Journal of Research in Personality, 40(5), 694-712. | [33] | Mar R. A., Oatley K., & Peterson J. B . (2009). Exploring the link between reading fiction and empathy: Ruling out individual differences and examining outcomes. Communications, 34(4), 407-428. | [34] | Mar R. A., Tackett J. L., & Moore C . (2010). Exposure to media and theory-of-mind development in preschoolers. Cognitive Development, 25(1), 69-78. | [35] | Mildorf J . (2016). Reconsidering second-person narration and involvement. Language and Literature, 25(2), 145-158. | [36] | Mullins B., & Dixon P . (2007). Narratorial implicatures: Readers look to the narrator to know what is important. Poetics, 35(4-5), 262-276. | [37] | Mumper M. L., & Gerrig R. J . (2017). Leisure reading and social cognition: A meta-analysis. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 11(1), 109-120. | [38] | Oatley K . (1994). A taxonomy of the emotions of literary response and a theory of identification in fictional narrative. Poetics, 23(1-2), 53-74. | [39] | Oatley K . (1999). Meetings of minds: Dialogue, sympathy, and identification, in reading fiction. Poetics, 26(5-6), 439-454. | [40] | Panero M. E., Weisberg D. S., Black J., Goldstein T. R., Barnes J. L., Brownell H., & Winner E . (2016). Does reading a single passage of literary fiction really improve theory of mind? An attempt at replication. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 111(5), e46-e54. | [41] | Papeo L., Corradi-Dell’Acqua C., & Rumiati R. I . (2011). “She” is not like “I”: The tie between language and action is in our imagination. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23(12), 3939-3948. | [42] | Pino M. C., & Mazza M . (2016). The use of “literary fiction” to promote mentalizing ability. PLoS ONE, 11(8), e0160254. | [43] | Premack D., & Woodruff G . (1978). Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1(4), 515-526. | [44] | Rizzolatti G., Fadiga L., Gallese V., & Fogassi L . (1996). Premotor cortex and the recognition of motor actions. Cognitive Brain Research, 3(2), 131-141. | [45] | Speer N. K., Reynolds J. R., Swallow K. M., & Zacks J. M . (2009). Reading stories activates neural representations of visual and motor experiences. Psychological Science, 20(8), 989-999. | [46] | Stemmer B . (2008). CHAPTER 17 - Neuropragmatics: Disorders and neural systems. In B. Stemmer & H. A. Whitaker (Eds.), Handbook of the neuroscience of language. Amsterdam, the Netherlands: Elsevier. | [47] | Thompson J. M., Teasdale B., Duncan S., van Emde Boas E., Budelmann F., Maguire L., & Dunbar R. I. M . (2018). Individual differences in transportation into narrative drama. Review of General Psychology, 22(2), 210-219. | [48] | Vaughn L. A., Hesse S. J., Petkova Z., & Trudeau L . (2009). “This story is right on”: The impact of regulatory fit on narrative engagement and persuasion. European Journal of Social Psychology, 39(3), 447-456. | [49] | Völlm B. A., Taylor A. N. W., Richardson P., Corcoran R., Stirling J., McKie S., ... Elliott R . (2006). Neuronal correlates of theory of mind and empathy: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study in a nonverbal task. Neuroimage, 29(1), 90-98. |
|