[1] | 黄玺, 梁宏宇, 李放, 陈世民, 王巍欣, 林妙莲, 郑雪 . ( 2018). 道德提升感: 一种提升道德情操的积极道德情绪. 心理科学进展, 26, (7), 1253-1263. | [2] | 黄好, 罗禹, 冯廷勇, 李红 . ( 2010). 厌恶加工的神经基础. 心理科学进展, 18, (9), 1449-1457. | [3] | 李鸿展 . ( 2014). 近墨者黑: 不道德传染对道德判断的影响 (硕士学位论文). 广西师范大学. | [4] | 李琼, 刘力 . ( 2011). 低地位群体的外群体偏好. 心理科学进展, 19, (7), 1061-1068. | [5] | 廖敏 . ( 2012). 道家文化的当代心理学价值及其实现. 社会心理科学,( 9), 107-114. | [6] | 王晓辰, 高欣洁, 郭攀博 . ( 2018). 亲组织不道德行为的多层次模型. 心理科学进展, 26, (6), 1111-1120. | [7] | 吴宝沛, 张雷 . ( 2012). 厌恶与道德判断的关系. 心理科学进展, 20, (2), 309-316. | [8] | 夏天生, 徐雪梅, 李贺, 俞梦霞, 莫雷 . ( 2018). 洗掉不愉快的经历, 留住幸福的时刻——回忆情绪事件对人们清洗/保留倾向的影响. 心理科学, 41, (1), 105-111. | [9] | 徐细雄, 刘星 . ( 2013). 放权改革、薪酬管制与企业高管腐败. 管理世界, 3, 119-132. | [10] | 阎书昌 . ( 2011). 身体洁净与道德. 心理科学进展, 19, (8), 1242-1248. | [11] | 叶浩生 . ( 2011). 有关具身认知思潮的理论心理学思考. 心理学报, 43, (5), 589-598. | [12] | 叶红燕, 张凤华 . ( 2015). 从具身视角看道德判断. 心理科学进展, 23, (8), 1480-1488. | [13] | Albas D., &Albas C. ( 1989). Modern magic: The case of examinations. The Sociological Quarterly, 30, (4), 603-613. | [14] | Bem D.J . ( 1972). Constructing cross-situational consistencies in behavior: Some thoughts on Alker's critique of Mischel. Journal of Personality, 40, (1), 17-26. | [15] | Caldwell B. M., Harenski C. L., Harenski K. A., Fede S. J., Steele V. R., Koenigs M. R., & Kiehl K. A . ( 2015). Abnormal frontostriatal activity in recently abstinent cocaine users during implicit moral processing. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 9, 565. | [16] | Carlston D .( 2010). Models of implicit and explicit mental representation. In B. Gawronski & K. Payne (Eds.), Handbook of implicit social cognition: Measurement, theory and applications. (pp. 38-61). Guilford Press. | [17] | Chapman H. A., Kim D. A., Susskind J. M., & Anderson A. K . ( 2009). In bad taste: Evidence for the oral origins of moral disgust. Science, 323, (5918), 1222-1226. | [18] | Choi I., Nisbett R. E., & Norenzayan A . ( 1998). Causal attribution across cultures: Variation and universality. Psychological Bulletin, 125, (1), 47-63. | [19] | Danovitch J., &Bloom P. ( 2009). Children's extension of disgust to physical and moral events. Emotion, 9, (1), 107-112. | [20] | Diener E., &Seligman M. E.P . ( 2004). Beyond money toward an economy of well-being. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 5, (1), 1-31. | [21] | Ferguson M. A., &ranscombe B, N. R .( 2014) . The social psychology of collective guilt. In C. von Scheve, & M. Salmela (Eds.), Collective emotions: Perspectives from psychology, philosophy, and sociology (pp. 251-265) . Oxford, UK: Oxford Uni-versity Press. | [22] | Fowler J.H., &Christakis N.A . ( 2009). Dynamic spread of happiness in a large social network: Longitudinal analysis of the Framingham heart study social network. BMJ: British Medical Journal, 338, (7685), 23-27. | [23] | Eskine K. J., Kacinik N. A., & Webster G. D . ( 2012). The bitter truth about morality: Virtue, not vice, makes a bland beverage taste nice. Plos One, 7, (7), e41159. | [24] | Eskine K. J., Novreske A., & Richards M . ( 2013). Moral contagion effects in everyday interpersonal encounters. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 49, (5), 947-950. | [25] | Gámez E., Díaz J. M., & Marreo H . ( 2011). The uncertain universality of the Macbeth effect with a Spanish sample. Spanish Journal of Psychology, 14, (1), 156-162. | [26] | Gausel N. ( 2012). Facing in-group immorality: Differentiating expressed shame from expressed guilt. Review of European Studies, 4, (4), 1-7. | [27] | Giner-Sorolla R., Kupfer T., & Sabo J . ( 2018). What makes moral disgust special? An integrative functional review. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 57, 223-289. | [28] | Gino F., Gu J., & Zhong C-B . ( 2009). Contagion or restitution? When bad apples can motivate ethical behavior. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45, (6), 1299-1302. | [29] | Graham J., Haidt J., Koleva S., Motyl M., Iyer R., Wojcik S. P., & Ditto P. H.. ( 2013). Moral foundations theory: The pragmatic validity of moral pluralism. In P. Devine & A. Plant (Eds.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 47, pp. 55-130). Burlington, MA: Academic Press. | [30] | Grappi S., Romani S., & Bagozzi R. P . ( 2013). Consumer response to corporate irresponsible behavior: Moral emotions and virtues. Journal of Business Research, 66, (10), 1814-1821. | [31] | Greene J., &Haidt A.J . ( 2002). How (and where) does moral judgment work?. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6, (12), 517-523. | [32] | Greene J. D., Morelli S. A., Lowenberg K., Nystrom L. E., & Cohen J. D . ( 2008). Cognitive load selectively interferes with utilitarian moral judgment. Cognition, 107, (3), 1144-1154. | [33] | Haidt J., Rozin P., Mccauley C., & Imada S . ( 1997). Body, psyche, and culture: the relationship between disgust and morality. Psychology and Developing Societies, 9, (1), 107-131. | [34] | Hanna M.B., &Marcus H. ( 2016). Individual differences in moral development: Does intelligence really affect children’s moral reasoning and moral emotions?. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 1961. | [35] | Hardy S.A., &Carlo G. ( 2011). Moral identity: What is it, how does it develop, and is it linked to moral action?. Child Development Perspectives, 5, (3), 212-218. | [36] | Harenski C.L., &Hamann S. ( 2006). Neural correlates of regulating negative emotions related to moral violations. Neuroimage, 30, (1), 313-324. | [37] | Hejmadi A., Rozin P., & Siegal M . ( 2004). Once in contact, always in contact: Contagious essence and conceptions of purification in American and Hindu Indian children. Developmental Psychology, 40, (4), 467-476. | [38] | Helzer E.G., &Pizarro D.A . ( 2011). Dirty liberals!: Reminders of physical cleanliness influence moral and political attitudes. Psychological Science, 22, (4), 517-522. | [39] | Ho Y-H., &Lin C-Y. ( 2014). The moral judgment relationship between leaders and followers: A comparative study across the Taiwan strait. Journal of Business Ethics, 134, (2), 299-310. | [40] | Hood B. M., Gjersoe N. L., Donnelly K., Byers A., & Itajkura S . ( 2011). Moral contagion attitudes towards potential organ transplants in British and Japanese adults. Journal of Cognition & Culture, 11, (3/4), 269-286. | [41] | Inbar Y., &Pizarro D. ( 2013). Pollution and purity in moral and political judgment. Advances in Experimental Moral Psychology, 111-129. | [42] | Inbar Y., Pizarro D. A., Knobe J., & Bloom P . ( 2009). Disgust sensitivity predicts intuitive disapproval of gays. Emotion, 9, (3), 435-439. | [43] | Jong sung L., Dae hee K., & Braunstein-minkove J . ( 2016). Coping with athlete endorsers' immoral behavior: Roles of athlete identification and moral emotions on moral reasoning strategies. Journal of Sport Management, 30, (2), 176-191. | [44] | Koleva S. P., Graham J., Iyer R., Ditto P. H., & Haidt J . ( 2012). Tracing the threads: How five moral concerns (especially purity) help explain culture war attitudes. Journal of Research in Personality, 46, (2), 184-194. | [45] | Lakens D .( 2014). Grounding social embodiment. Social Cognition, 32, 168-183. | [46] | Lee A., &Ji L.J . ( 2014). Moving away from a bad past and toward a good future: Feelings influence the metaphorical understanding of time. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 143, (1), 21-26. | [47] | Lee S. W.S., &Schwarz N. ( 2010). Washing away postdecisional dissonance. Science, 328, (5979), 709-709. | [48] | Leffel G.M . ( 2008). Who cares? Generativity and the moral emotions, part 2: A "social intuitionist model" of moral motivation. Journal of Psychology & Theology, 36, (3), 182-201. | [49] | Legare C. H., Wellman H. M., & Gelman S. A . ( 2009). Evidence for an explanation advantage in naïve biological reasoning. Cognitive Psychology, 58, (2), 177-194. | [50] | Leutgeb V., Wabnegger A., Leitner M., Zussner T., Scharmüller W., Klug D., & Schienle A . ( 2016). Altered cerebellar-amygdala connectivity in violent offenders: A resting-state fMRI study. Neuroscience Letters, 610, 160-164. | [51] | Levav J., &McGraw A.P . ( 2009). Emotional accounting: How feelings about money influence consumer choice. Journal of Marketing Research, 46, (1), 66-80. | [52] | Marczyk J. .( 2015). Moral alliance strategies theory. Evolutionary Psychological Science, 1, (2), 77-90. | [53] | Martin J. W., &Cushman F .(in press). The adaptive logic of moral luck. A Companion to Experimental Philosophy. | [54] | Mauss M. ( 1972). A general theory of magic (R. Brain, Trans.). New York: W. W. Norton. ( Original work published 1902) | [55] | Mcdermott E., &Langdon P.E . ( 2016). The moral reasoning abilities of men and women with intellectual disabilities who have a history of criminal offending behaviour. Legal and Criminological Psychology, 21, (1), 25-40. | [56] | Moll J., de Oliveira-Souza R., Moll F. T., Ignácio F. A., Bramati I. E., Caparelli-Dáquer E. M., & Eslinqer P. J . ( 2005). The moral affiliations of disgust: A functional MRI study. Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology, 18, (1), 68-78. | [57] | Nemeroff C., & Rozin P. ( 2000) . The makings of the magical mind: The nature and function of sympathetic magical thinking In K S Rosengren, C N Johnson, & P L Harris (Eds), Imagining the impossible: Magical, scientific, and religious thinking in children (pp 1-34) New York: Cambridge University Press . | [58] | Nemeroff C., &Rozin P. ( 1994). The contagion concept in adult thinking in the United States: Transmission of germs and of interpersonal influence. Ethos, 22, (2), 158-186. | [59] | Newman G.E., &Bloom P. ( 2014). Physical contact influences how much people pay at celebrity auctions. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 111, (10), 3705-3708. | [60] | Olatunji B. O., Cisler J., McKay D., & Phillips M. L . ( 2010). Is disgust associated with psychopathology? Emerging research in the anxiety disorders. Psychiatry Research, 175, (1-2), 1-10. | [61] | Paxton J.M., &Greene J.D . ( 2010). Moral reasoning: Hints and allegations. Topics in Cognitive Science, 2, (3), 511-527. | [62] | Pham M.T . ( 2006). Emotion and rationality: A critical review and interpretation of empirical evidence. Review of General Psychology, 11, (2), 155-178. | [63] | Pivetti M., Camodeca M., & Rapino M . ( 2016). Shame, guilt, and anger: Their cognitive, physiological, and behavioral correlates. Current Psychology, 35, (4), 690-699. | [64] | Rai T.S., &Fiske A.P . ( 2011). Moral psychology is relationship regulation: Moral motives for unity, hierarchy, equality, and proportionality. Psychological Review, 118, (1), 57-75. | [65] | Rohrmann S., &Hopp H. ( 2008). Cardiovascular indicators of disgust. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 68, (3), 201-208. | [66] | Rozin P. ( 1999). The process of moralization. Psychological Science, 10, (3), 218-221. | [67] | Rozin P., Haidt J., & Fincher K . ( 2009). From oral to moral. Science, 323, (5918), 1179-1180. | [68] | Rozin P., Haidt J., & Mc Cauley C . ( 2000). Disgust. In M. Lewis & J. M. Haviland (Eds.), Handbook of emotions( 2nd ed., pp. 637-653). New York: Guilford. | [69] | Rozin P., Markwith M., & Mccauley C . ( 1994). Sensitivity to indirect contacts with other persons: AIDS aversion as a composite of aversion to strangers, infection, moral taint, and misfortune. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 103, (3), 495-504. | [70] | Rozin P., Markwith M., & Nemeroff C . ( 1992). Magical contagion beliefs and fear of AIDS. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 22, (14), 1081-1092. | [71] | Rozin P., &Royzman E.B . ( 2001). Negativity bias, negativity dominance, and contagion. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 5, (4), 296-320. | [72] | Sanfey A. G., Rilling J. K., Aronson J. A., Nystrom L. E., & Cohen J. D . ( 2003). The neural basis of economic decision-making in the Ultimatum Game. Science, 300, (5626), 1755-1758. | [73] | Schaich Borg J., Lieberman D., & Kiehl K. A . ( 2008). Infection, incest, and iniquity: Investigating the neural correlates of disgust and morality. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20, (9), 1529-1546. | [74] | Schnall S., Haidt J., Clore G. L., & Jordan A. H . ( 2008). Disgust as embodied moral judgment. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34, (8), 1096-1109. | [75] | Schur A . ( 2008). Beyond moral evangelism: On the rejection of punishment in Late Tolstoy. Tolstoy Studies Journal, 20, 38. | [76] | Selterman D., Moors A. C., & Koleva S . ( 2018). Moral judgment toward relationship betrayals and those who commit them. Personal Relationships, 25 (1), 65-86. | [77] | Sharvit K., Brambilla M., Babush M., & Colucci F. P . ( 2015). To feel or not to feel when my group harms others? The regulation of collective guilt as motivated reasoning. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 41, (9), 1223-1235. | [78] | Sherman G.D., &Clore G.L . ( 2010). The color of sin: White and black are perceptual symbols of moral purity and pollution. Psychological Science, 20, (8), 1019-1025. | [79] | Simpson J., Carter S., Anthony S. H., & Overton P. G . ( 2006). Is disgust a homogeneous emotion?. Motivation and Emotion, 30, (1), 31-41. | [80] | Smetana J.G . ( 1995). Morality in context: Abstractions, ambiguities and applications. In R. Vasta (Ed.), Annals of child development ( Vol 10, pp. 83-130). London: Jessica Kingsley Publishers. | [81] | Smith E.R . ( 1993). Chapter 13-Social identity and social emotions: Toward new conceptualizations of prejudice. Affect Cognition and Stereotyping, 297-315. | [82] | Smith E. R., Seger C. R., & Mackie D. M . ( 2007). Can emotions be truly group level? Evidence regarding four conceptual criteria. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93, (3), 431-446. | [83] | Stellar J.E., &Willer R. ( 2013). The corruption of value: Negative moral associations diminish the value of money. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 5, (1), 60-66. | [84] | Tangney J. P., Stuewig J., & Mashek D. J . ( 2006). Moral emotions and moral behavior. Annual Review of Psychology, 58, 345-372. | [85] | Tapp C., &Occhipinti S. ( 2016). The essence of crime: Contagious transmission from those who have committed moral transgressions. British Journal of Social Psychology, 55, (4), 756-772. | [86] | Tasimi A., &Gelman S.A . ( 2016). Dirty money: The role of moral history in economic judgments. Cognitive Science, 41, (S3), 523-544. | [87] | Travis R., &Fergus T.A . ( 2015). The potentiating effect of disgust sensitivity on the relationship between disgust propensity and mental contamination. Journal of Obsessive-Compulsive and Related Disorders, 6, 114-119. | [88] | Umphress E. E., Bingham J. B., & Mitchell M. S . ( 2010). Unethical behavior in the name of the company: The moderating effect of organizational identification and positive reciprocity beliefs on unethical pro-organizational behavior. Journal of Applied Psychology, 95, (4), 769-780. | [89] | Xie W., Yu B., Zhou X., Sedikides C., & Vohs K. D . ( 2014). Money, moral transgressions, and blame. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 24, (3), 299-306. | [90] | Ying X. P., Luo J., Chiu C. Y., Wu Y. H., Xu Y., & Fan J . ( 2018). Functional dissociation of the posterior and anterior insula in moral disgust. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 860. | [91] | Zanin A. C., Bisel R. S., & Adame E. A . ( 2016). Supervisor moral talk contagion and trust-in-supervisor: mitigating the workplace moral mum effect. Management Communication Quarterly, 30, (2), 147-163. | [92] | Zhang X., Guo Q., Zhang Y., Lou L., & Ding D . ( 2015). Different timing features in brain processing of core and moral disgust pictures: An event-related potentials study. Plos One, 10, (5), e0128531. | [93] | Zhong C.B., &Liljenquist K. ( 2006). Washing away your sins: Threatened morality and physical cleansing. Science, 313, (5792), 1451-1452. | [94] | Zhong C-B., Strejcek B., & Sivanathan N . ( 2010). A clean self can render harsh moral judgment. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 46, (5), 859-862. |
|